




版权说明:本文档由用户提供并上传,收益归属内容提供方,若内容存在侵权,请进行举报或认领
文档简介
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20251
HowACTMathematicsPerformanceIsRelatedtoClassroomSmartphoneDistractionsand
MathematicsAnxiety
JeffSchiel
Summary
Recentresultsfromlarge-scaletestingprograms(e.g.,theACT®test,Programmefor
InternationalStudentAssessment)showdeclinesovertimeinhighschoolstudents’
mathematicsperformance.Suchdeclinescanbeconcerningformany,includingstudents,
parents,andeducators.Severalfactorscouldberelatedtomathematicsperformancedeclines,includingtheCOVID-19pandemic,technologydistractionsinmathematicsclasses,and
mathematicsanxiety.ThisstudyinvestigatedtherelationshipbetweenACTmathematicsscoresandtwofactors:distractionsfromsmartphonesinmathematicsclassesandmathematics
anxiety.
Keyfindingsfromthestudyaredescribedbrieflybelow.
Distractionsfromsmartphonesinmathematicsclasses—whetherfromastudent’sownsmartphoneorfromthesmartphonesofotherstudents—werefoundtohaveasignificantnegativerelationshipwithsubsequentperformanceontheACTmathematicstest,evenwhenthestudycontrolledforimportantstudentbackgroundvariables(e.g.,gender,
race/ethnicity,highschoolGPA,numberofhighschoolmathematicscoursestaken).
MathematicslearninganxietyandmathematicsevaluationanxietywerebothsignificantlyandnegativelyrelatedtoACTmathematicsperformance.Thiswasevidentevenwhenstudentbackgroundvariableswerecontrolledfor.
Mathematicsisnottheonlysubjectforwhichdomain-specificanxietymightbeofconcern.Performanceinscience,asmeasuredbytheACTsciencetest,wasalsofoundtobe
significantlyandnegativelyrelatedtosciencelearninganxiety.
Introduction
Studentperformanceinmathematicsappearstohavedeclinedinthepastfewyears.Some
evidenceofthiscomesfromthe2022ProgrammeforInternationalStudentAssessment(PISA)assessment,whichwasadministeredtonearly700,00015-year-oldstudentsin81countriestoassessperformanceinreading,science,andespeciallymathematics.Itwasfoundthatthe
averagemathematicsscorehaddecreasedbyalmost15pointssincethelastassessment,
whichwasin2018.Thisdecreaseisunprecedented;until2022,nochangeintheaveragehaseverexceeded4points(OECD,2023).
CT
©2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20252
CT
©2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435
IntheirreportofthePISAresults,theOrganisationforEconomicCo-operationand
DevelopmentstatedthatthedropinmathematicsscorepointstotheshockeffectofCOVID-19onmostcountries(OECD,2023,p.27).Theyalsostated,however,thatthepandemicmightnotbetheonlyfactorinvolved,becausescienceperformancedidnotchangesignificantly,onaverage,between2018and2022.TheaveragePISAsciencescoredecreasedbyonly2pointsduringthisperiod.
Anotherhypothesisforthedeclineinmathematicsperformancewasthattechnologyis
distractingteenagersduringtheirmathematicsclasses.PISAfoundthatstudentswhoreportedbecomingdistractedinmostorallmathematicslessonsscoredanaverageof15pointslowerinmathematicsthandidstudentswhoreportedthatthisneveroralmostneverhappened(OECD,2023).Otherstudiessupportthishypothesis,havingfoundnegativerelationshipsbetween
tertiarystudentssmartphoneuseandtheiracademicperformance.AsummaryofthefindingsofseveralsuchstudiesispresentedbyAmezandBaert(2020).
ACTmathematicsscoreshavealsodeclinedovertime.In2019,forexample,theaveragescorewas20.4.In2023,itwas19.0(ACT,2023).UnlikePISAsciencescores,however,theACT
sciencescorehasdeclinedaswell(20.6in2019,19.6in2023).DeclinesinACTscoresappeartobesmallerthanthoseinPISAscoresbecauseofthedifferentscalesusedbythetwo
assessments.TheACTscorescalerangesfrom1to36,andadeclineof1or2pointsinan
averagescorecanbesubstantive.Incomparison,PISAscorestheoreticallyhavenominimumormaximumbutinsteadarescaledtofitdistributionsthatareapproximatelynormal,withmeansaround500scorepointsandstandarddeviationsaround100scorepoints(OECD,2023).
Therefore,similar1-or2-pointdeclinesinPISAscoresgenerallyarenotsubstantive.
FactorsthatmighthavebeenexacerbatedbytheCOVID-19pandemic,suchasmathematicsanxiety,mightalsoberelatedtothemathematicsperformancedecline.Mathematicsanxietyhasbeenshowntoberelatedbothtodecreasedmathematicsperformance(Richardson&
Suinn,1972)andtoaffectedself-confidenceandclearthinkingwhenfacingamathematicsproblem,especiallyforfemalehighschoolstudents(Escalera-Chávezetal.,2017).
TheCOVID-19pandemicwasfoundtoberelatedtoasignificantincreaseinthelevelof
mathematicsanxietyinasampleofcollegestudents(Soysaletal.,2022).Inaddition,limited
accesstotechnologyandaninabilitytocommunicateadequatelywithinstructorswererelated
toincreasesinmathematicsanxietyamongcollegestudentsfollowingapandemic-driven
emergencytransitiontoremotelearning(Laniusetal.,2022).PerhapsthepandemiccontributedinasimilarwaytoincreasedmathematicsanxietyandtobothACTandPISAmathematics
scoredeclinesamonghighschoolstudents.
Thepurposeofthisstudywastoexplore1)therelationshipbetweentechnologydistractionsinmathematicsclasses,specificallythoseresultingfromtheuseofsmartphones,and
performanceontheACTmathematicstestand2)therelationshipbetweenmathematicsanxietyandperformanceontheACTmathematicstest.Inaddition,todeterminewhethersmartphonedistractionsandanxietymightberelatedtoperformanceinsubjectsotherthanmathematics,
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20253
CT
©2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435
thisstudyexploredrelationshipsamongsmartphonedistractionsinscienceclasses,scienceanxiety,andperformanceontheACTsciencetest.
Thedataforthisstudywereobtainedfromasampleofhighschoolstudentswhohadtaken
eithertheDecember2023ortheFebruary2024nationalACTtestorhadregisteredfor,butnotyettaken,theApril2024test.InApril2024,thesestudentswereaskedseveralquestions
designedtoevaluate1)howdistractedtheywerebytheirownorotherstudentssmartphonesduringmathematicsandscienceclassesand2)thedegreeoftheirmathematicsandscienceanxiety.
1
Itwashypothesizedthatthedatawouldindicateanegativerelationshipbetweenmathematics
anxietyandACTmathematicsperformance(i.e.,higherlevelsofanxietywouldberelatedto
lowerlevelsofperformance)andasimilarnegativerelationshipbetweensmartphone
distractionsinmathematicsclassesandACTmathematicsperformance(i.e.,higherlevelsof
distractionwouldberelatedtolowerlevelsofperformance).Similarfindingswereanticipatedforscience.
Findings
ACTMathematicsPerformanceandSmartphoneDistractionsinMathematicsClasses
DistractionsinMathematicsClassesFromStudents’OwnSmartphones
Studentswereaskedhowoftentheyweredistractedduringtheirmathematicsclassesbytheirownsmartphones.MeanACTmathematicsscoresdecreasedasthefrequencyofsmartphonedistractionsincreased(Figure1).
2
Forexample,themeanscoreofstudentswhoreportedthattheywereneveroralmostneverdistractedbytheirphones(22.8)washigherthanthatof
studentswhoreportedthattheywerealmostalwaysdistractedbytheirphones(20.3).Analphalevelof.01wasusedinthisstudyforstatisticaltestsofdifferencesbetweenmeanscores.
Althoughthedifferencebetweenthesetwomeanscoreswasnotsignificantaccordingtothiscriterion(q=3.10,p=.0104),thepatternofdecreasesdisplayedbythemeanssuggeststhattherewasanegativerelationshipbetweenstudentsperformanceontheACTmathematicstestandthefrequencyofdistractionsfromtheirsmartphonesduringmathematicsclasses.A
regressionanalysis,whichisdescribedlaterinthissection,confirmedthis.
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20254
Figure1.MeanACTMathematicsScorebyFrequencyofDistractionsinMathematicsClassesFromStudents’OwnSmartphones
MeanACTMathematicsScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
22.8
22.1
21.6
20.3
Neveroralmostnever
(n=4,119)
SometimesOften
(n=2,339)(n=561)
HowOftenDistractedByYourSmartphone
Almostalways
(n=258)
DistractionsinMathematicsClassesFromtheSmartphonesofOtherStudents
Studentswereaskedhowoftentheyweredistractedduringtheirmathematicsclassesbyotherstudents’smartphones.Similartowhatwasobservedfordistractionsfromstudents’own
smartphones,meanACTmathematicsscorestendedtodecreaseasthefrequencyof
distractionsfromotherstudents’smartphonesincreased(Figure2).Anunexpectedfindingwasthatthemeanofthe“almostalways”levelwassomewhatlargerthanthatofthe“often”level
(20.5versus19.8,respectively;thedifferencebetweenthesemeanswasnotstatistically
significant;q=0.83,p=.8401).Apossibleexplanationisthatstudentswhosaidthattheywerealmostalwaysdistractedbyotherstudents’smartphonesduringmathematicsclasseshad
adjustedwelltofrequentdistractionsandthuswereabletolearnmathematicalconceptsaswellasthosestudentswhoreportedsomewhatfewerdistractions.Itisimportanttokeepinmindthattheinterpretationoffindingsforthisdistractionlevelmightbelimitedbecauseofitsrelatively
smallsamplesize(n=183).
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20255
Figure2.MeanACTMathematicsScorebyFrequencyofDistractionsinMathematicsClassesFromtheSmartphonesofOtherStudents
MeanACTMathematicsScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
22.8
22.4
20.5
19.8
NeveroralmostneverSometimesOftenAlmostalways
(n=4,810)(n=1,738)(n=451)(n=183)
HowOftenDistractedByOtherStudents'Smartphones
Anotherexplanationisthattherewasadisproportionatenumberofhigh-achievingstudentsinthe“almostalways”distractionlevelandthattheywereabletofocusinmathematicsclasses
irrespectiveofthefrequencyofsmartphonedistractions.Thispossibilityissupportedbyastudythatexaminedtheacademicperformanceofhighschoolstudentsfollowingmobilephonebansintheirschools.Thatstudy’sfindingssuggestedboththathigh-achievingstudentswereabletofocusintheclassroomwhethermobilephoneswerebannedornotandthattheywere
unaffectedbyanynegativeimpactsofmobilephoneuse(Beland&Murphy,2016).However,thecurrentstudyfoundnoevidencethattherewasadisproportionatenumberofhighachieversinthe“almostalways”level(studentachievementlevelwasdeterminedbyenrollmentin
advancedplacement,accelerated,orhonorscoursesinmathematics;thenumberofmathematicscoursestaken;andhighschoolGPA).
Incaseschool-widesmartphonebansweresomehowrelatedtothisfinding,theproportionsofstudentswhoseschoolshadbannedsmartphones(seeQuestion5intheSurveyInstrument
sectionofthetechnicalappendix)werecomparedacrossdistractionlevels.Theproportionsdidnotdiffersignificantly.Moreover,itwasfoundinaregressionmodelforpredictingACT
mathematicsperformance—amodelthatincludedavariablefordistractionsfromother
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20256
students’smartphones,avariableforschool-widesmartphonebans,andvariablesrelatedtostudentbackground—thattheregressioncoefficientforschool-widesmartphonebanswasnotstatisticallysignificant(b=−0.71,t=−2.45,p=.0144).
3
ThissuggeststhatsmartphonebanswerenotrelatedtoACTmathematicsperformanceanddistractionsfromotherstudents’
smartphoneswhenstudentbackgroundvariableswerecontrolledfor.
Thereisanotherpossibilitywemightconsider.Perhapsthisunexpectedfindinghassomethingtodowiththescaleusedforthisquestion.Thatis,ifstudentshaddifficultydistinguishing
betweenthe“often”and“almostalways”levels,thenthedifferencebetweenthemeansfor
theselevelscouldbetheresultofmeasurementerror.However,asimilarfindingdidnotoccurforthequestionaboutdistractionsfromstudents’ownsmartphonesinmathematicsclasses,whichusedthesamescale.Thus,measurementerrorseemsanunlikelyexplanation.
ExpectedDecreasesinACTMathematicsScoreasaFunctionofSmartphoneDistractionsinMathematicsClassesandStudentBackgroundVariables
AregressionanalysiswasusedtofurtherexploretherelationshipbetweenACTmathematicsscoreandsmartphonedistractions.ACTmathematicsscorewasmodeledasafunctionofthefrequencyofdistractionsinmathematicsclassesfromeitherstudents’ownsmartphonesorthesmartphonesofotherstudents,plusseveralstudentbackgroundvariablesknowntoberelatedtoACTmathematicsscore.Thesevariablesincludedgender,race/ethnicity,familyincome
category,highschoolGPA,numberofmathematicscoursestaken,andwhetherastudentwasenrolledinadvancedplacement,accelerated,orhonorscoursesinmathematics.
FindingsoftheregressionanalysisindicatedthattherewouldbeanexpecteddecreaseinACTmathematicsscoreofapproximately−0.42scalescorepointsforeachunitchangeinlevelof
distractionfromastudent’sownsmartphonewhenstudentbackgroundvariableswere
controlledfor.Forexample,ifthelevelofdistractionfromastudent’ssmartphonewereto
increasefrom“neveroralmostnever”to“almostalways,”anincreaseofthreeunits,thenthe
expecteddecreaseinACTmathematicsscorewouldbe−1.26scalescorepoints(b=−0.42,t=−2.59,p=.0097;seeTableA2,Model1,inthetechnicalappendix).
TheexpecteddecreaseinACTmathematicsscorerelatedtodistractionsfromthesmartphonesofotherstudentswaslargerthanthatrelatedtodistractionsfromstudents’ownsmartphones:approximately−0.70scalescorepointsforeachunitchangeinlevelofdistraction.Forexample,ifthelevelofdistractionfromotherstudents’smartphonesweretoincreasefrom“neveror
almostnever”to“almostalways,”thentheexpectedmathematicsscoredecreasewouldbe−2.10scalescorepoints(b=−0.70,t=−4.11,p<.0001;TableA2,Model2).Clearly,thesefindingsindicateanegativerelationshipbetweensmartphonedistractionsandACT
mathematicstestperformance,evenwhentheeffectsofimportantstudentbackgroundvariablesarecontrolledfor.
ACTMathematicsPerformanceandMathematicsAnxiety
Toevaluatetheextentoftheirmathematicsanxiety,studentswereadministeredthe
AbbreviatedMathAnxietyScale(AMAS;Hopkoetal.,2003).TheAMASconsistsofnineitems,
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20257
fiveofwhichmeasureanxietyrelatedtolearningmathematics(LMA)andfourofwhichmeasuremathematicsevaluationanxiety(MEA).TheLMAsubscalescorerangesfrom5to25,andtheMEAsubscalescorerangesfrom4to20.AdditionalinformationabouttheAMAScanbefoundintheSurveyInstrumentsectionofthetechnicalappendix.
LearningMathematicsAnxiety
MeanACTmathematicsscoresdecreasedasthelevelofanxietyrelatedtolearning
mathematicsincreased(Figure3).Forexample,studentswithrelativelylowanxiety(i.e.,an
LMAsubscalescoreof5or6,whichwasthebottomquarteroftheLMAscoredistributionforstudentsinthisstudy)hadameanmathematicsscoreof24.6,whereasstudentswithrelativelyhighanxiety(i.e.,anLMAsubscalescorebetween13and25,thetopquarter)hadamean
mathematicsscoreof19.2.Thedifferencebetweenthesemeanswasstatisticallysignificant(q=14.87,p<.0001,d=1.01).
4
Figure3.MeanACTMathematicsScorebyLevelofLearningMathematicsAnxiety
MeanACTMathematicsScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
24.6
23.7
22.4
19.2
5–6
(n=1,972)
7–8
(n=1,146)
9–12
(n=1,620)
13–25
(n=1,338)
LearningMathematicsAnxietySubscaleScore
MathematicsEvaluationAnxiety
Asimilar,negativerelationshiptothatbetweenmathlearninganxietyandACTmathematicsperformancewasobservedforanxietyrelatedtomathematicsevaluation.MeanACT
mathematicsscoresdecreasedasevaluationanxietyincreased,witha3.7scalescorepoint
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20258
CT
©2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435
differenceinmeanscores(q=8.62,p<.0001,d=0.65)betweenstudentswithrelativelylowanxiety(i.e.,anMEAsubscalescorebetween4and10,thebottomquarteroftheMEAscoredistribution)andthosewithrelativelyhighanxiety(i.e.,anMEAsubscalescoreof19or20,thetopquarter;Figure4).
Figure4.MeanACTMathematicsScorebyLevelofMathematicsEvaluationAnxiety
MeanACTMathematicsScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
24.3
23.0
22.1
20.6
410
(n=1,561)
11141518
(n=1,544)(n=1,958)
MathematicsEvaluationAnxietySubscaleScore
1920
(n=1,063)
ExpectedDecreasesinACTMathematicsScoreasaFunctionofMathematicsAnxietyandStudentBackgroundVariables
MultiplelinearregressionwasusedtomodelACTmathematicsscoreasafunctionof
mathematicsanxiety(LMAsubscaleorMEAsubscale)andstudentbackgroundvariables
relatedtoperformanceonthistest(gender,race/ethnicity,familyincomecategory,highschoolGPA,numberofmathematicscoursestaken,andwhetherastudentwasenrolledinadvancedplacement,accelerated,orhonorscoursesinmathematics).Thefindingsindicatedthattherewouldbeanexpecteddecreaseinmathematicsscoreofapproximately−0.32scalescore
pointsforeachunitincreaseinLMAsubscalescore.Forexample,ifastudentslevelofLMAweretoincreasefrom8(themedianscoreforstudentsinthisstudy)to12(the75thpercentile),thentheexpecteddecreaseinmathematicsscorewouldbe−1.28scalescorepoints(b=
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March20259
CT
©2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435
−0.32,t=−9.75,p<.0001;TableA2,Model3),evenwhenstudentbackgroundvariableswerecontrolledfor.
MultipleregressionfindingsformathematicsevaluationanxietyindicatedanegativerelationshipbetweenthismeasureandACTmathematicsperformance.ForeachunitincreaseinMEA,
therewasanexpecteddecreaseinmathematicsscoreofapproximately−0.24scalescorepoints(b=−0.24,t=−7.22,p<.0001;TableA2,Model4)whengender,race/ethnicity,andotherstudentbackgroundvariableswerecontrolledfor.
ACTSciencePerformanceandSmartphoneDistractionsinScienceClasses
TheprimarygoalofthisstudywastoexaminetherelationshipsbetweenACTmathematicsscoresandtwofactors:smartphonedistractionsinmathematicsclassesandmathematicsanxiety.Anothergoalwastodeterminewhethersmartphonedistractionsandanxietywererelatedtoperformanceinsubjectsotherthanmathematics.ThissectionpresentsfindingsofnegativerelationshipsbetweenACTscienceperformanceandtwofactors:smartphone
distractionsinscienceclassesandscienceanxiety.
5
DistractionsinScienceClassesFromStudents’OwnSmartphones
Asthefrequencyofdistractionsinscienceclassesincreased,ACTsciencescoresdecreasedonaverage.Forexample,themeansciencescoreofstudentswhoreportedthattheywere
neveroralmostneverdistractedbytheirownsmartphonesinscienceclasseswaslargerthanthatofstudentswhoreportedthattheywerealmostalwaysdistractedinthismanner(23.6
versus21.6,respectively;Figure5).However,thesemeansdidnotdiffersignificantlyfromeachother(q=2.80,p=.0266).
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March202510
CT
©2025byACTEducationCorp.Allrightsreserved.|R2435
Figure5.MeanACTScienceScorebyFrequencyofDistractionsinScienceClassesFromStudents’OwnSmartphones
26
MeanACTScienceScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
23.623.423.3
21.6
Neveroralmostnever
(n=3,910)
Sometimes
(n=2,204)
Often
(n=750)
Almostalways
(n=233)
HowOftenDistractedByYourSmartphone
DistractionsinScienceClassesFromtheSmartphonesofOtherStudents
MeanACTsciencescorestendedtodecreaseasthefrequencyofdistractionsfromthe
smartphonesofotherstudentsinscienceclassesincreased.Forexample,themeanscience
scoreofstudentswhoreportedarelativelylowfrequencyofsmartphonedistractionwashigherthanthatofstudentswhoreportedarelativelyhighfrequency(meanfor“neveroralmostnever”level=23.5versus22.4for“almostalways”level;Figure6).However,thedifferencebetweenthesemeanswasnotstatisticallysignificant(q=1.35,p=.5295).
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March202511
Figure6.MeanACTScienceScorebyFrequencyofDistractionsinScienceClassesFromtheSmartphonesofOtherStudents
MeanACTScienceScore(99%ConfidenceInterval)
26
25
24
23
22
21
20
19
18
23.8
23.5
22.7
22.4
NeveroralmostneverSometimesOftenAlmostalways
(n=4,672)(n=1,686)(n=501)(n=213)
HowOftenDistractedByOtherStudents'Smartphones
Themeansciencescoreofstudentsinthe“sometimes”levelwasslightlyhigherthanthatof
studentsinthe“neveroralmostnever”level(23.8versus23.5,respectively;thedifference
betweenthesemeanswasnotstatisticallysignificant;q=0.88,p=.8153).Thehypothesis
proposedbyBelandandMurphy(2016)thathigh-achievingstudentsareabletofocusinthe
classroomandareunaffectedbyanynegativeimpactsofmobilephoneusewasconsideredasapossibleexplanationforthisunexpectedfinding.Severalmeasuresofstudentachievement
wereexaminedacrosssmartphonedistractionlevels.Itwasfoundthatthepercentageof
studentsenrolledinadvancedplacement,accelerated,orhonorscoursesinthenatural
sciences;thenumberofnaturalsciencescoursestaken;andhighschoolGPAwerecomparableacrosslevels.Therefore,itisunlikelythatthedifferencesinmeanACTsciencescoresacrossthedistractionlevelswererelatedtodifferencesinproportionsofhigh-achievingstudents.
School-widesmartphonebansdidnotappeartoberelatedtothisunexpectedfindingeither.
Theproportionsofstudentswhoseschoolshadimplementedsmartphonebansdidnotdiffersignificantlyacrossdistractionlevels.Inaddition,aregressionanalysisindicatedthat
smartphonebanswerenotsignificantlyrelatedtoACTsciencescore(b=一0.46,t=一1.51,p=.1313)whilecontrollingforsmartphonedistractionsinscienceclassesandstudentbackground
ACTResearch|IssueBrief|March202512
variables(gender,race/ethnicity,familyincomecategory,highschoolGPA,numberofnaturalsciencescoursestaken,andwhetherastudentwasenrolledinadvancedplacement,
accelerated,orhonorscoursesinthenaturalsciences).
ExpectedDecreasesinACTScienceScoreasaFunctionofSmartphoneDistractionsinScienceClassesandStudentBackgroundVariables
RegressionmodelsweredevelopedtofurtherexaminetherelationshipbetweenACTscienceperformanceandsmartphonedistractionswhilecontrollingforgender,race/ethnicity,family
incomecategory,highschoolGPA,numberofnaturalsciencescoursestaken,andwhetherastudentwasenrolledinadvancedplacement,accelerated,orhonorscoursesinthenatural
sciences.Unliketheregressioncoefficientsforthemathematicsmodels,however,the
regressioncoefficientsforthesciencemodelswerenotstatisticallysignificant(b=一0.20,t=
一1.12,p=.2617fordistractionsfromstudents’ownsmartphonesandb=一0.07,t=一0.37,p=.7092fordistractionsfromthesmartphonesofothers),thusprecludingacceptableestimationofdecreasesinACTsciencescorethatwouldbeexpectedforunitchangesinthesefactors.
Moreover,thesefindingssuggestthattherelationshipsbetweenACTscienceperformanceandsmartphonedistractionsinscienceclasseswerestatisticallyweakerthanthosebetweenACTmathematicsperformanceandsmartphonedistractionsinmathematicsclasses.
ACTSciencePerformanceandScienceAnxiety
Tomeasurestudents’levelsofscienceanxiety,theywereadministeredtheAbbreviated
ScienceAnxietyScale(ASAS;Megreyaetal.,2021),whichisadaptedfromthemodified
AbbreviatedMathAnxietyScale(m-AMAS;Careyetal.,2017).Them-AMASisadaptedfromtheAMAS.IncreatingtheASAS,theauthorsadjustedthem-AMASbyreplacingmathematics-relatedwordingwithscience-relatedwording.LiketheAMAS,thenine-itemASASyieldstwo
subscalescores,oneforlearningscienceanxiety(LSA;5items,withscoresrangingfrom5to25)andoneforscienceevaluationanxiety(SEA;4items,withscoresrangingfrom4to20).AdditionalinformationabouttheASAScanbefoundintheSurveyInstrumentsectionofthetechnicalappendix.
LearningScienceAnxiety
HighermeanACTsciencescoreswererelatedt
温馨提示
- 1. 本站所有资源如无特殊说明,都需要本地电脑安装OFFICE2007和PDF阅读器。图纸软件为CAD,CAXA,PROE,UG,SolidWorks等.压缩文件请下载最新的WinRAR软件解压。
- 2. 本站的文档不包含任何第三方提供的附件图纸等,如果需要附件,请联系上传者。文件的所有权益归上传用户所有。
- 3. 本站RAR压缩包中若带图纸,网页内容里面会有图纸预览,若没有图纸预览就没有图纸。
- 4. 未经权益所有人同意不得将文件中的内容挪作商业或盈利用途。
- 5. 人人文库网仅提供信息存储空间,仅对用户上传内容的表现方式做保护处理,对用户上传分享的文档内容本身不做任何修改或编辑,并不能对任何下载内容负责。
- 6. 下载文件中如有侵权或不适当内容,请与我们联系,我们立即纠正。
- 7. 本站不保证下载资源的准确性、安全性和完整性, 同时也不承担用户因使用这些下载资源对自己和他人造成任何形式的伤害或损失。
最新文档
- 山东菏泽郓城重点达标名校2025年初三练习题二(全国卷II)语文试题含解析
- 吉林省普通高中联合体2025年高三物理试题4月质量调研测试(二模)试题含解析
- 浙江省教育考试院2024-2025学年高三第三次模拟生物试题含解析
- 员工绩效评估合同模板
- 合同收据格式
- 电磁兼容测试高级工程师聘请协议
- 二手住宅交易协议合同
- 地铁线路建设工程施工合同协议
- 促进创业和小型企业在阿曼支持经济多样化的研究:阿曼
- 一种替来他明制备工艺方法的改进及中试研究
- 雪铁龙DS6说明书
- 智慧农业中的农业无人机技术与应用
- 玻璃瓶丝印制度
- 中医养生如何调理肺炎
- 屋顶花园设计方案
- pzt压电陶瓷制备工艺
- 中班游戏教案《背夹球》
- 第5课《小心“马路杀手”》课件
- 零星维修工程投标方案技术标
- 森林消防员劳务派遣服务投标方案技术标
- 妇科学妇科感染病
评论
0/150
提交评论