




版权说明:本文档由用户提供并上传,收益归属内容提供方,若内容存在侵权,请进行举报或认领
文档简介
1、Institute for Research on Labor and Employment UC Berkeley Title: Cultural Effects on Employee Loyalty in Japan and The U. S.: Individual- or Organization-Level? An Analysis of Plant and Employee Survey Data from the 80s Author: Lincoln, James R., University of California, Berkeley Doerr, Bernadette
2、, University of California, Berkeley Publication Date: 01-04-2012 Series: Working Paper Series Publication Info: Working Paper Series, Institute for Research on Labor and Employment, UC Berkeley Permalink: /uc/item/8sc9k91b Keywords: Employee Loyalty, Japan, United States Abstr
3、act: This paper uses 1980s survey data on large samples of American and Japanese factories and their employees to examine how organization (factory) cultures then differed between Japan and the U. S. and how they affected employee loyalty - intention to leave or stay. Central to the analysis is the
4、idea, taken from Blaus seminal 1962 paper, that cultural effects may operate at the individual-level through the values, beliefs, and norms employees accept and “internalize” but also at the group- (including organization-) level through the mechanism of social pressure aimed at inducing conformity.
5、 Following Benedicts classic attribution of a “shame” culture to Japan and “guilt” culture to the U. S., we predict and find that cultural dimensions pertaining to company paternalism/familism and group work shape employee loyalty chiefly at the organization-level in Japan and chiefly at the individ
6、ual-level in the U. S. This conclusion is qualified, however, by the finding that in both countries the “strength” (within-plant variance) of the culture conditions the size of the cultural effects. They are larger when the culture is stronger. Apart from question of the level at which cultural effe
7、cts operate, we find, consistent with most expectations, that Japanese employees are more loyal (that is, less inclined to quit) in the presence of organization cultures favoring paternalism/familism, groupism, and vertical cohesion (close/frequent supervision). The reverse is in general true of the
8、 American employees. eScholarship provides open access, scholarly publishing services to the University of California and delivers a dynamic research platform to scholars worldwide. CULTURAL EFFECTS ON EMPLOYEE LOYALTY IN JAPAN AND THE U. S.: INDIVIDUAL- OR ORGANIZATION-LEVEL? An Analysis of Plant a
9、nd Employee Survey Data from the 80s James R. Lincoln () Bernadette Doerr () Walter A. Haas School of Business University of California, Berkeley 1 January, 2012 2 CULTURAL EFFECTS ON EMPLOYEE LOYALTY IN JAPAN AND THE U. S.: INDIVIDUAL- OR ORG
10、ANIZATION-LEVEL? An Analysis of Plant and Employee Survey Data from the 80s ABSTRACT This paper uses 1980s survey data on large samples of American and Japanese factories and their employees to examine how organization (factory) cultures then differed between Japan and the U. S. and how they affecte
11、d employee loyalty - intention to leave or stay. Central to the analysis is the idea, taken from Blaus seminal 1962 paper, that cultural effects may operate at the individual-level through the values, beliefs, and norms employees accept and “internalize” but also at the group- (including organizatio
12、n-) level through the mechanism of social pressure aimed at inducing conformity. Following Benedicts classic attribution of a “shame” culture to Japan and “guilt” culture to the U. S., we predict and find that cultural dimensions pertaining to company paternalism/familism and group work shape employ
13、ee loyalty chiefly at the organization-level in Japan and chiefly at the individual-level in the U. S. This conclusion is qualified, however, by the finding that in both countries the “strength” (within-plant variance) of the culture conditions the size of the cultural effects. They are larger when
14、the culture is stronger. Apart from question of the level at which cultural effects operate, we find, consistent with most expectations, that Japanese employees are more loyal (that is, less inclined to quit) in the presence of organization cultures favoring 3 paternalism/familism, groupism, and ver
15、tical cohesion (close/frequent supervision). The reverse is in general true of the American employees. 4 INTRODUCTION Culture- values, beliefs, normsthat emerge spontaneously within a group or community and ideologiessimilar forms but consciously devised by one group or stratum for the purpose of in
16、fluencing the thinking and actions of othersshape the attitudes and behaviors of individuals within those groups and communities in two ways. First, individuals accept as their own, presumably through a socialization and identification process (which may be anticipatory of actual entry or selection
17、into the group), the cultural content of the group. This process of individual acceptance and internalization of cultural elements can come about through two broad mechanisms. One is a micro-process, operating on individuals. It subsumes the following: (1) the individual is deterministically sociali
18、zed by the group; (2) the individual self-selects into a group whose cultural patterns are akin to those s/he had previously embraced; (3) the individual interactively has a hand in creating the group via the ties s/he develops with others and to whom, in turn, s/he passes on his or her values and b
19、eliefs. Through each of the above channels, the individual comes to internalize and thus personally accept and identify with the culture/ideology, consciously or not, and consequently thinks, feels, and acts on it. The second distinct mechanism through which cultures/ ideologies condition individual
20、s attitudes and behavior is of a very different sort. There is no presumption here that the focal person has internalized the culture of the group- woven it into his or her sense 5 of self. Rather, the “cultural effect” comes from the pressures to which s/he is subjected from those members who have
21、so internalized it to act or comport him/herself in ways consistent with it. The canonical example in the sociology literature of such an effect appears in Durkheims classic study of suicide (Durkheim, 1966). For reasons having to do with doctrines of sin but also with individual choice his data sho
22、wed Protestants killing themselves more frequently than individual Catholics. But Protestants residing in predominantly Catholic countries were less inclined to suicide than were their counterparts in predominantly Protestant countries. Such pressures from others to conform with cultural patterns to
23、 which the individual did not personally subscribe or accept is the primary and most obvious ways in which culture can be said to have an existence that is outside or separate from the values, beliefs, and sentiments of individual persons. An early paper by Peter Blau (1962) was the first to address
24、 the problem in and apply Durkheims methods to a formal organizational setting. Blau observed that the behavior of case workers in an employment agency varied both with their own individual values and beliefs regarding the treatment of clients some more pro-client, others leaning pro-agency (in term
25、s of minimizing costs, expediting throughput, etc.) but also with the representation of such values and beliefs among their coworkers. Blau reported on a number of distinct configurations of such group- and individual-level orientations. In some, the client orientation of the group supplemented or a
26、ugmented the orientation of the caseworker. In others, the two 6 effects shifted behavior in opposite directions. Blau labeled the phenomenon he observed a “structural effect,” although his only conceptualization and measurement of “structure” was the attachment of the individual caseworker to his o
27、r her professional colleagues within the agency. “Cultural effect” is arguably a better label for what he observed, as both his theory and his data spoke to how the values, beliefs, and norms of groupsin Blaus research human service agencies conditioned the behavior of their members. Of course, as n
28、oted above, the influences of culture on individuals actions and orientations may operate through individual-level mechanisms such as the socialization and selection and creation or institutionalization processes noted above. Indeed, most contemporary research and theorizing on organizational cultur
29、e by social psychologists focus on the internalization of and thus sharing by a set of individuals of cultural contents. What was structural in the effects analyzed by Blau was not the cultural or ideological content but the mechanismpeer pressure or peer pressure or social influencethat seemed to m
30、odify behaviors directly without being mediated by individuals hearts and minds. Blaus “structural effects” are known in the sociological and education literatures as “contextual or “compositional” effects but they are also referred to, particularly in a recent and influential stream of modeling, as
31、 “exogenous social effects” (Manski, 1993). By contrast, an “endogenous social effect,” also termed a “contagion effect,” concerns the much 7 less empirically tractable influence on an individuals behavior of the distribution (typically average) of that same behavior in a group of which that individ
32、ual is a member or, more broadly, a network of others to which s/he is tied. Beginning with the “Coleman report” in the 1960s on educational opportunity in the U. S. (Coleman et al., 1966), a very large number of studies by sociologists, educational psychologists, and economists have investigated en
33、dogenous as well as exogenous effects of schools, classes, and grades on student academic achievement. Another sizable body of multidisciplinary work examines neighborhood and peer group effects on crime and poverty (Quigley and Raphael, 2008). An important stream of organizational research, most of
34、 it framed by neo-institutional theory, examines contagion as the mechanism whereby an innovative organizational form or practice diffuses through an organizational population, field, or network (Burt, 1987; Davis, 1991).1 Most recently a series of high-profile studies by 1 The difference between “g
35、roup” effects of the sort Blau studied and “network” effects of the sort Burt, Davis, and Christakis and Fowler have studied is really a small one both conceptually and in terms of the modeling involved (Erbring and Young, 1979; Friedkin, 1990). The usual exogenous/endogenous “group effects” model r
36、elating an individual-level response variable to the group averages of one or more individual-level explanatory variables is in fact a special case of the more general “network effect” models wherein the matrix mapping egos ties to alters contains blocks of cells that are all “1” (in the group) or a
37、ll “0” (out of the group). The network effects model allows for each ego to be tied to his/her own “group” (ego network) (Friedkin, 1990). 8 public health scholars have examined social contagion effects on obesity, divorce, smoking, even mental/emotional states such as happiness. All these streams o
38、f research address important, interesting, and intuitively compelling issues in how people and organizations influence one another and all, particularly in recent years, have come in for a great deal of tough methodological criticism (for a sampling see, e.g., Manski, 1993; Moffitt, 2001; Shalizi an
39、d Thomas, 2011; VanderWeele, 2011). Despite the large cross-disciplinary literature addressed in general to exogenous and endogenous social effects in a variety of problem areas, since Blaus canonical piece there has been next to no subsequent research on the specific problem that interested him: ho
40、w the distribution of values, norms, and beliefs among a set of persons might through group and network mechanisms condition and channel those persons attitudes and behaviors (for an exception that uses the contextual effects modeling apparatus of the time see Lincoln and Zeitz, 1980). Yet the quest
41、ion that concerned Blau is still a very timely and important one for student of organizational behavior: the extent to which the culture of a group, network, organization, even community or society determines the attitudes and behaviors its members through a process of social influence or pressure t
42、o conform as opposed to a process (emphasized in most of the organizational culture literature to date) of individuals through socialization coming to share the culture of the group by psychologically internalizing it and 9 identifying with it as their own individual system of values and beliefs.2 T
43、his is the focus of the present study. The present study: cultural effects in Japanese and U. S. factories in the 80s We study cultural effects as contextual effects using a unique data set collected in the 1980s on over 100 Japanese and U. S. manufacturing plants and representative samples of their
44、 employees. The role played by culture in forming the attitudes and behaviors of individuals takes on multiple overlapping dimensions in a study of how American and Japanese factory employees are motivated by the cultures and social structures of their countries, the cultures, structures, and compos
45、itions of their companies, and the jobs, ranks, training levels and types, and demographics that that differentiate them within those companies. One of the earliest and most famous characterizations of how the motivational 2 A sizable number of recent studies, sometimes using laboratory and simulati
46、on techniques, have examined the related question of how whereby cultures emerge and take shape in organizations as a function of their members composition and networks. (Carroll and Harrison, 1998; Berger and Luckman, 1966; Frank and Fahrbach, 1999; Carley and Hill, 2001; Krackhardt and Kilduff; 20
47、02; Lincoln and Guillot, 2006). However, because of their conceptual and technical complexity and their attention to dynamics these models are of relative limited utility as guides to the nonexperimental and often cross-sectional empirical research that comprises the bulk of the social effects liter
48、ature. 10 constraints and drivers (see Vaisey, 2009) of culture diverge between Japan and the West culture is Ruth Benedicts (1946) classic distinction between “guilt” and “shame” cultures. For Benedict, drawing heavily as did sociologist Parsons on the fashionable Freudian thinking of the time, soc
49、ialization infuses into Westerners hearts and minds ethical/normative codes, such that people feel pangs of conscience-guilt-when they stray from the directions of their moral compasses. In Japanese shame culture, by contrast, behavior is guided, less by the normative programming acquired through th
50、e nurture, upbringing, conditioning, etc., more by how others react when it fails to meet (or perhaps exceeds) their expectaions. Blaus structural effects article did not take up the question of cross-national differences, but he framed the problem for his study with a very similar distinction: “The
51、 common values and norms in a group have two distinct kinds of effect upon the conduct of its members. Ego's conduct is influenced by his own normative orientation for fear of his conscience, and ego's conduct is also influenced by alters' normative orientation for fear of social sanctio
52、ns. In other words, people conform to prevailing norms partly because they would feel guilty if they did not and partly because they gain social approval and avoid disapproval by doing so.” Thus, for Blau, the client- versus bureaucratic- orientations of professional staff in an American social serv
53、ices agency might be influence either through an individual-level 11 mechanism (“guilt), a group-level mechanism (“shame;” i.e., social pressure and sanctions) approval), oras some his findings testifiedsome interaction of the two. Yet for Benedict and later generations of cross cultural social psyc
54、hologists (Markus and Kitayama, 1991), the individual-level “guilt” mechanism whereby cultural values and beliefs bear on individual attitudes and behavior operate is more prevalent in the Anglo-American West, whereas the group-level “shame“ mechanism predominates in Japan. A seeming weakness in the
55、 Benedicts attribution of the “shame” mechanism to Japan is that it is not obvious how a shame culture can come about unless some sizable number of persons has internalized and thus genuinely feels and “believes in” the cultural codes they seek to impose on others. Benedicts theory attributes to Jap
56、an a sociologically interesting pattern in which no one feels in his/her heart of hearts that a course of action is the right one but in sensing that it is the normatively correct one they participate willingly in efforts to pressure and sanction nonconformists.3 “Group think” comes to mind, amusingly 3 An interesting experimental design study by Willer, Kuwabara, and Mach (2009) addresses the question of why people
温馨提示
- 1. 本站所有资源如无特殊说明,都需要本地电脑安装OFFICE2007和PDF阅读器。图纸软件为CAD,CAXA,PROE,UG,SolidWorks等.压缩文件请下载最新的WinRAR软件解压。
- 2. 本站的文档不包含任何第三方提供的附件图纸等,如果需要附件,请联系上传者。文件的所有权益归上传用户所有。
- 3. 本站RAR压缩包中若带图纸,网页内容里面会有图纸预览,若没有图纸预览就没有图纸。
- 4. 未经权益所有人同意不得将文件中的内容挪作商业或盈利用途。
- 5. 人人文库网仅提供信息存储空间,仅对用户上传内容的表现方式做保护处理,对用户上传分享的文档内容本身不做任何修改或编辑,并不能对任何下载内容负责。
- 6. 下载文件中如有侵权或不适当内容,请与我们联系,我们立即纠正。
- 7. 本站不保证下载资源的准确性、安全性和完整性, 同时也不承担用户因使用这些下载资源对自己和他人造成任何形式的伤害或损失。
最新文档
- 工业管道的维护与检修方法
- 工作中的自我管理与激励方法
- 工业设计与科技创新的融合发展
- 工业风味的文化创意街区转型实践
- 工业风建筑设计理念与实践
- 工业设计产业园在服务领域的应用
- 工程中的液压传动系统设计与分析
- 工厂企业消防安全管理体系
- 工程机械设备的技术改造与升级
- 工程教育中数据科学的课程设计
- 核辐射物理学基础知识课件
- 陶瓷装饰基础模拟题参考答案 中国地质大学
- 分词作定语的用法课件
- 中小企业管理成长诊断课件
- 《学术英语(综合)》课后答案
- 计算材料学:第一性原理计算及其应用课件
- 部编一年级《咕咚》课本剧
- 天津市新版就业、劳动合同登记名册
- 会议管理系统的分析与设计
- 水电工程投标文件(技术标书)
- 《作物栽培学》课件-第八章甘蔗栽培
评论
0/150
提交评论